Children participation to negative and positive events and resistance to suggestibility

Journal title MALTRATTAMENTO E ABUSO ALL’INFANZIA
Author/s Paola Di Blasio, Chiara Ionio
Publishing Year 2024 Issue 2023/3 Suppl. Language Italian
Pages 24 P. 63-86 File size 313 KB
DOI 10.3280/MAL2023-003-S1005
DOI is like a bar code for intellectual property: to have more infomation click here

Below, you can see the article first page

If you want to buy this article in PDF format, you can do it, following the instructions to buy download credits

Article preview

FrancoAngeli is member of Publishers International Linking Association, Inc (PILA), a not-for-profit association which run the CrossRef service enabling links to and from online scholarly content.

Research studies on suggestibility have shown both how the ability of children to resist suggestive questions increases when they are the protagonists and not the mere observers of the event. Our research is part of this line of studies; it has the objective of analyzing whether resistance to suggestibility is favoured either by participation in the event or by the emotive connotation of the event. The research protocol was realized in three phases. In the first phase, the participants experienced three different events (neutral; negative-participated and positive-participated) in which they were involved as protagonists. After one week and after one month, the children were first asked to perform a free re-evocation and later interrogated on the three events by 27 questions (non-suggestive, misleading and tag). The participants at first and second phases were 124 children of between 7 and 10 years (average age = 8.56), of whom 58 were girls and 66 boys. At thirth phase participated 71 children. The results show significant differences in resistance to misleading and tag ques-tions, which is connected to the type of event. In particular, after one week and one month, the questions on the negative-participated event received the greatest number of correct answers and the lowest number of incorrect answers, in connection with both misleading and tag ques-tions.

Keywords: suggestibility, participation, emotional connotation, scholar children.

  1. Goodman, G. S., & Reed, R. S. (1986). Age differences in eyewitness testimony. Law and Human Behavior, 10, 317-332.
  2. Ackil, J. K., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1995). Developmental differences in eyewitness suggestibility and memory for source. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60(1), 57-83.
  3. Aldridge, M., & Wood, J. (1998). Interviewing children: A guide for child care and forensic practitioners. New York: Wiley.
  4. Alexander, K. W., Goodman, G. S., Schaaf, J. M., Edelstein, R. S., Quas, J. A., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). The role of attachment and cognitive inhibition in children’s memory and suggestibility for a stressful event. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 262-290.
  5. Berliner, L., Hyman, I., Thomas, A., & Fitzgerald, M. (2003). Children’s memory for trauma and positive experiences. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16(3), 229-236.
  6. Binet, A. (1900). La suggestibilitè. Paris: Schleicher Freres.
  7. Brady, M. S., Poole, D. A., Warren, A. R., & Jones, H. R. (1999). Young children’s responses to yes-no questions: Patterns and problems. Applied Developmental Science, 3(1), 47-57.
  8. Bright-Paul, A., Jarrold, C., & Wright, D. B. (2008). Theory-of-mind development influences suggestibility and source monitoring. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1055-1068.
  9. Bruck, M., & Melnyk, L. (2004). Individual differences in children’s suggestibility: A review and synthesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(8), 947-996.
  10. Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children’s memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 419-439.
  11. Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Mechanism of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 294-299.
  12. Casciano, M., Mazzoni, G., & De Leo, G. (2004). Falsi ricordi indotti da informazioni fuorvianti e da interviste ripetute sulla memoria di eventi non accaduti. Maltrattamento e abuso all’infanzia, 6(3), 39-60.
  13. Cassel, W. S., Roebers, C. E. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1996). Developmental patterns of eyewitness responses to repeated and increasingly suggestive questions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61(2), 116-133.
  14. Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1993). The suggestibility of the child witness: A historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 403-439.
  15. Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scientific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  16. Ceci, S. J., Ross, D. F., & Toglia, M. P. (1987). Suggestibility of children’s memory: Psycholegal implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 38-49.
  17. Ceci, S. J., Kulkofsky, S., Klemfuss, J. Z., Sweeney, C. D., & Bruck, M. (2007). Unwarranted assumptions about children’s testimonial accuracy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 311-328.
  18. Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A., & Bryant, B. (2003). Cognitive predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder in children: results of a prospective longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(1), 1-10.
  19. Fivush, R., Hazzard, A., Sales, J. M., Sarfati, D., & Brown, T. (2003). Creating coherence out of chaos? Children’s narratives of emotionally negative and positive events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 1-19.
  20. Gobbo, C., & Raccanello, D. (2007). How Children Narrate Happy and Sad Events: does Affective State Count? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1-18.
  21. Gobbo, C., Mega, C., & Pipe, M. E. (2002). Does the nature of the experience influence suggestibility? A study of children’s event memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 502-530.
  22. Goodman, G. (1984). The child witness: Conclusions and future directions. Journal of Social Issues, 40, 157-175.
  23. Goodman, G. S., Hirschman, J. E., Hepps, D., & Rudy, L. (1991). Children’s memory for stressful events. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 37, 109-158.
  24. King, M., & Yuille, J. (1987). Suggestibility and the child witness. In S. J. Ceci, M. Toglia & D. Ross (eds). Children’s eyewitness memory. New York: Springer-Verlag.
  25. Koriat, A., Goldsmith, M., Schneider, W., & Nakash-Dura, M. (2001). The credibility of children’s testimony: Can children control the accuracy of their memory reports? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 405-437.
  26. Krackow, E., & Lynn, S. J. (2003). Is there touch in the game of Twister? The effects of innocuous touch and suggestive questions on children’s eyewitness memory. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 589-604.
  27. Kulkofsky, S., & Klemfuss, J. Z. (2008). What the stories children tell can tell about their memory: Narrative skill and young children’s suggestibility. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1442-1456.
  28. Lamb, M. E., & Fauchier, A. (2001). The effects of question type on self-contradictions by children in the course of forensic interviews, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15(5), 483-491.
  29. Lamb, M. E., Orbach, Y., Hershkowitz, I., Horowitz, D. & Abbott, C. B. (2007). Does the type of prompt affect the accuracy of information provided by alleged victims of abuse in forensic interviews? Applied Cognitive Psychology Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1117-1130.
  30. Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). The effects of stereotypes and suggestions on preschoolers’ reports. Developmental Psychology, 31, 568-578.
  31. Loftus, E. F. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 560-572.
  32. Mazzoni, G. (2000). La testimonianza nei casi di abuso sessuale sui minori: la memoria, l’intervista e la validità della deposizione. Milano: Giuffrè.
  33. Mazzoni, G. (2003). Si può credere a un testimone?La testimonianza e le trappole della memoria. Bologna: il Mulino.
  34. McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M. (1985). Misleading postevent information and memory for events: Arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 1-16.
  35. Merritt, K. A., Ornstein, P. A., & Spicker, B. (1994). Children’s memory for a salient medical procedure: Implications for testimony. Pediatrics, 94, 17-23.
  36. Miragoli, S., & Stagni Brenca, E. (2009). Testimonianza e suggestionabilità in età prescolare: recenti sviluppi di ricerca. Età Evolutiva, 94, 118-128.
  37. Murachver, T., Pipe, M. E., Gordon, R., Owens, J. L., & Fivush, R. (1996). Do, show, and tell: Children’s event memories acquired through direct experience, observation, and stories. Child Development, 67, 3029-3044.
  38. Peterson, C. (1999). Children’s memories for medical emergencies: 2 years later. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1493-1506.
  39. Peterson, C., & Bell, M. (1996). Children’s memory for traumatic injury. Child Development, 67, 3045-3070.
  40. Peterson, C., & Biggs, M. (1998). Stitches and casts: Emotionally and narrative coherence. Narrative Inquiry, 8(1), 51-76.
  41. Peterson, C., & Whalen, N. (2001). Five years later: Children’s memories for medical emergencies. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1-18.
  42. Pezdek, K., & Roe, C. (1995). The effect of memory trace strength on suggestibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 116-128.
  43. Pezdek, K., & Taylor, J. (2001). Memories of traumatic events. In M. Eisen, G. S. Goodman & J. S. Quas (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview. (pp. 165-184) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  44. Pipe, M. E., Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., Bidrose, S., Ablin, D., & Craw, S. (1997). Remembering early experiences during childhood: Are traumatic events special? In D. Read & S. Lindsay (Eds.), Recollections of trauma: Scientific research and clinical practice (pp. 417-423). NY: Plenum.
  45. Pipe, M. E., & Salmon, K. (2002). What children bring to the interview context: Individual differences in children events reported. In M. Eisen, J. A. Quas & G. S. Goodman (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview (pp. 235-263). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  46. Poole, D. A., & Lindsay, D. S. (1995). Interviewing preschoolers: Effects of nonsuggestive techniques, parental coaching, and leading questions on reports of nonexperienced events. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, 405-437.
  47. Poole, D. A., & Lamb, M. E. (1998). Investigative interviews of children: A guide for helping professionals. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  48. Powell, M. B., & Roberts, K. P. (2002). The effect of repeated experience on children’s suggestibility across two question types. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 367-386.
  49. Quas, J. A, & Schaaf, J. M. (2002). Children’s memories of experienced and nonexperienced events following repeated interviews. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 304-338.
  50. Quas, J. A., Goodman, G. S., Bidrose, S., Pipe, M. E., Craw, S., & Ablin, D. S. (1999). Emotion and memory: children’s long-tem remembering, forgetting, and suggestibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 235-270.
  51. Redlich, A. D., & Goodman, G. S. (2003). Taking responsibility for an act not committed: The influence of age and suggestibility. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 141-156.
  52. Reisberg, D., & Hertel, P. (2004). Memory and emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  53. Roebers, C. M., & McConkey, K. M. (2003). Contextual reinstatement and the misinformation effect in children and adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 477-497.
  54. Roebers, C. M., & Schneider, W. (2000). The impact of misleading questions on eyewitness memory in children and adults. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 509-526.
  55. Roebers, C. M., Gelhaar, T., & Schneider, W. (2004). It’s magic! The effects of presentation modality on children’s event memory, suggestibility, and confidence judgments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 320-335.
  56. Rudy, L., & Goodman, G. S. (1991). Effects of participation on children’s reports: Implications for children’s testimony. Developmental Psychology, 27, 527-538.
  57. Sales, J., Fivush, R., Parker, J., & Bahrick, L. (2005). Stressing memory: Long-term relations among children’s stress, recall and psychological outcome following hurricane Andrew. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(4), 529-545.
  58. Schaaf, J. M., Alexander, K. W., & Goodman, G. S. (2008). Children’s false memory and true disclosure in the face of repeated questions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 100, 157-185.
  59. Schreiber, N., & Parker, J. F. (2004). Inviting witnesses to speculate: Effects of age and interaction on children’s recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(1), 31-52.
  60. Stern , W. (1910). Abstracts of lectures on the psychology of testimony and on the study of individuality. American Journal of Psychology, 21, 270-282.
  61. Sutherland, R., & Hayne, H. (2001). Age-related changes in the misinformation effect. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(4), 388-404.
  62. Sutherland, R., Pipe, M. E., Schick, K., Murray, J., & Gobbo, C. (2003). Knowing in advance: The impact of prior event information on memory and event knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(3), 244-263.
  63. Thierry, K. L., & Spence, M. J. (2004). Children’s memory and suggestibility for a real-life and video event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 297-309.
  64. Tobey, A., & Goodman, G. S. (1992). Children’s eyewitness memory: Effects of participation and forensic context. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 779-796.
  65. Vandermass, M. O., Hess, T. M., & Baker-Ward, L. (1993). Does anxiety affect children’s reports of memory for a stressful event? Journal of Applied Psychology, 7, 109-128.
  66. Varendonck, J. (1911). Les temoignages d’enfants dans un proces retentissant. Archives de Psycholgie, 11, 129-171.

Paola Di Blasio, Chiara Ionio, Partecipazione ad eventi negativi e positivi e resistenza alla suggestionabilità in "MALTRATTAMENTO E ABUSO ALL’INFANZIA" 3 Suppl./2023, pp 63-86, DOI: 10.3280/MAL2023-003-S1005