Folk psychiatry. La psichiatria fra immagine scientifica e psichiatria popolare

Titolo Rivista PSICOTERAPIA E SCIENZE UMANE
Autori/Curatori Andrea Angelozzi
Anno di pubblicazione 2022 Fascicolo 2022/3 Lingua Italiano
Numero pagine 26 P. 431-546 Dimensione file 120 KB
DOI 10.3280/PU2022-003004
Il DOI è il codice a barre della proprietà intellettuale: per saperne di più clicca qui

Qui sotto puoi vedere in anteprima la prima pagina di questo articolo.

Se questo articolo ti interessa, lo puoi acquistare (e scaricare in formato pdf) seguendo le facili indicazioni per acquistare il download credit. Acquista Download Credits per scaricare questo Articolo in formato PDF

Anteprima articolo

FrancoAngeli è membro della Publishers International Linking Association, Inc (PILA)associazione indipendente e non profit per facilitare (attraverso i servizi tecnologici implementati da CrossRef.org) l’accesso degli studiosi ai contenuti digitali nelle pubblicazioni professionali e scientifiche

La ricerca scientifica in psichiatria sta creando un divario nei confronti delle nozioni di psichiatria che posseggono le persone comuni, in maniera analoga alla progressiva distanza fra psicologia scientifica e psicologia popolare e di senso comune. Vengono indagate le nozioni che la psichia-tria scientifica e quella popolare condividono, e quelle in cui si differenziano spesso con aspetti controintuitivi. Dopo aver delineato i tratti essenziali del concetto di senso comune e di psicologia popolare e delle loro teorie più rappresentative, viene sottolineato il ruolo centrale che i concetti popolari svolgono non solo nella teoria psichiatrica, in particolare nella diagnosi, ma anche nella pratica quotidiana. Questi concetti entrano poi nella immagine di sé stessi, del mondo e nelle rela-zioni interpersonali, mostrando la improponibilità di una psichiatria che miri a fare a meno del senso comune e la necessità di una attenta mediazione fra psichiatria scientifica e psichiatria popolare. Questo aspetto è ancora più importante nella psichiatria pratica che si rivela simile per molti aspetti alla psichiatria popolare.;

Keywords:Psichiatria popolare; Psicologia popolare; Senso comune; Immagine scientifica; Modelli scientifici

  1. Churchland P.S. (1986). Neurophilosophy: Toward a Unified Science of the Mind-Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  2. Cruz J. & Gordon R.M. (2006). Simulation theory. In: Nadel L., editor, Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. London: Macmillan, 2006, pp. 1-6.
  3. Davidson D. (1974). Psychology as philosophy. In: Brown S.C., editor, Philosophy of Psychology. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. pp. 41-52.
  4. Davies M. & Stone T. (1998). Folk psychology and mental simulation. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplements, 43: 53-82. DOI: 10.1017/S1358246100004306
  5. Dawes R.M. (1994). House of Cards: Psychology and Psychotherapy Built on Myth. New York: Free Press.
  6. Deikman A.J. (1966). De-automatization and the mystic experience. Psychiatry, 29, 4: 324-338. DOI: 10.1080/00332747.1966.11023476
  7. Ennis B.J. & Litwack T.R. (1974). Psychiatry and the presumption of expertise: Flipping coins in the courtroom. California Law Review, 62, 3: 693-752. DOI: 10.15779/Z38XX8W
  8. Eysenck H.J. (1953). Sense and Nonsense in Psychology. London: Penguin (trad. it.: Senso e controsenso in psicologia. Firenze: Giunti-Barbera, 1961).
  9. Agazzi E., a cura di (2004). Valore e limiti del senso comune. Milano: FrancoAngeli.
  10. Ahn W.K., Novick L.R. & Kim N.S. (2003). Understanding behavior makes it more normal. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 3: 746-752.
  11. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Washington, D.C.: APA (trad. it.: DSM-5. Manuale diagnostico e statistico dei disturbi mentali. Quinta edizione. Milano: Raffaello Cortina, 2014).
  12. Andrews K. (2007). It’s in your nature: A pluralistic folk psychology. Synthèse, 165: 13-29.
  13. Angelozzi A. (2006). Psicoterapia, suggestione e senso comune. Psicoterapia, X, 30, 1: 263-267.
  14. Angelozzi A. (2021a). L’ipotesi dello spostamento nel suicidio: persona e situazione in psicopatologia. Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, 55, 2: 247-264. DOI: 10.3280/PU2021-002004
  15. Angelozzi A. (2021b). Problemi della previsione in psichiatria. Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, 55, 4: 623-646. DOI: 10.3280/PU2021-004005.
  16. Angermeyer M.C. & Matschinger H. (1996). Public attitude towards psychiatric treatment. Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica, 94, 5: 326-336.
  17. Angermeyer M.C. & Matschinger H. (1999). Lay beliefs about mental disorders: A comparison between the western and the eastern parts of Germany. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 34, 5: 275-281.
  18. Angermeyer M.C., Matschinger H. & Schomerus G. (2013). Attitudes towards psychiatric treatment and people with mental illness: Changes over two decades. British Journal of Psychiatry, 203, 2: 146-151.
  19. Angermeyer M.C., van der Auwera S., Carta M.G. & Schomerus G. (2017). Public attitudes towards psychiatry and psychiatric treatment at the beginning of the 21st century: A systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys. World Psychiatry, 16, 1: 50-61.
  20. Batchelor I.R. (1963). Psychiatry and common sense. Scottish Medical Journal, 8, 11: 413-419. DOI: 10.1177/003693306300801101
  21. Baron-Cohen S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (trad. it.: L’autismo e la lettura della mente. Roma: Astrolabio, 1997).
  22. Baumeister R.F., Bratslavsky E., Muraven M. & Tice D.M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 5: 1252-1265. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252
  23. Beretta C. (2019). “Tra folk psychology e fenomenologia. Sulla comprensione di persone e personaggi”. Tesi di Laurea al Corso Magistrale in Scienze Filosofiche Università di Padova: www.academia.edu/39238309/Tra_Folk_Psychology_e_Fenomenologia_Sulla_comprensione_di_persone_e_personaggi.
  24. Berger P.L. & Luckman T. (1967). The Social Construction of Reality. London: Penguin (trad. it.: La realta come costruzione sociale. Bologna: Il Mulino, 1985).
  25. Blankenburg W. (1971). Der Verlust der natürlichen Selbstverständlichkeit. Ein Beitrag zur Psychopathologie symptomarmer Schizophrenien. Stuttgart: Enke (trad. it.: La perdita dell’evidenza naturale: Un contributo alla psicopatologia delle schizofrenie pauci-sintomatiche. Milano: Raffaello Cortina, 1998).
  26. Boyer P. (2011). Intuitive expectations and the detection of mental disorder: A cognitive background to folk-psychiatries. Philosophical Psychology, 24, 1: 95-118, DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2010.529049
  27. Bruner J. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  28. Buttelmann D., Carpenter M. & Tomasello M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112, 2: 337-342.
  29. Carruthers P. (1996). Simulation and self-knowledge: A defence of the theory-theory. In: Carruthers & Smith, 1996, pp. 22-38.
  30. Carruthers P. & Smith P., editors (1996). Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  31. Catty J., Lissouba P., White S., Becker T., Drake R., Fioritti A., Knapp M., Lauber C., Rössler W., Tomov T., van Busschbach J., Wiersma D. & Burns T. (2008). Predictors of employment for people with severe mental illness: Results of an international six-centre randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 3: 224-231.
  32. Chartrand T.L. & Bargh J.A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 6: 893-910. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893
  33. Churchland P.M. (1981). Eliminative materialism and the propositional attitudes. Journal of Philosophy, 78, 2: 67-90. DOI: 10.2307/2025900
  34. Feyerabend P. (1963). Mental events and the brain. Journal of Philosophy, 60, 11: 295-296. DOI: 10.2307/2023030
  35. Fitzpatrick R. (1984) Lay concepts of illness: In: Fitzpatrick R., Hinton J., Newman S., Scambler G. & Thompson J., editors, The Experience of Illness. London: Tavistock, 1984, pp. 11-30.
  36. Fletcher G.J. (1984). Psychology and common sense. American Psychologist, 39, 3: 203-213. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.39.3.203
  37. Fletcher G.J. (1995). The Scientific Credibility of Folk Psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  38. Fodor J.A. (1968). The appeal to tacit knowledge in psychological explanation source. Journal of Philosophy, 65, 20: 627-640. DOI: 10.2307/2024316
  39. Fodor, J.A. (1992). A theory of the child’s theory of mind. Cognition, 44, 3: 283-296. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90004-2
  40. Fodor J.A. (1987). Psychosemantics. The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (trad. it. Psicosemantica. Il problema del significato nella filosofia della mente. Bologna: Il Mulino, 1990).
  41. Foucault M. (1961). Folie et déraison: histoire de la folie à l’âge classique. Paris: Plon (trad. it.: Storia della follia. Milano: Rizzoli, 1963, 1973)
  42. Freud S. (1916 [1917]). Una difficoltà della psicoanalisi. Opere, 8: 657-664. Torino: Boringhieri, 1976.
  43. Fuchs T. (2001). The tacit dimension. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology 8, 4: 323-326.
  44. Furnham A. (1984). Lay conceptions of neuroticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 1: 95-103. DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(84)90142-9
  45. Furnham A. (1988). Lay theories: Everyday Understanding of Problems in the Social Sciences. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.
  46. Furnham A. & Henderson M. (1982). The good, the bad and the mad: Response bias in self-report measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 3, 3: 311-320. DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(82)90051-4
  47. Furnham A. & Henderson M. (1983). The mote in thy brother’s eye, and the beam in thine own: Predicting one’s own and others’ personality test scores. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 3: 381-389.
  48. Furnham A., & Henley S. (1988). Lay beliefs about overcoming psychological problems. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, 3/4: 423-438.
  49. Furnham A. & Telford K. (2011). Public attitudes, lay theories and mental health literacy: The understanding of mental health. In: Abate L., editor, Mental Illnesses. Understanding, Prediction and Control. Rijeka (Croatia): Intech, 2011, pp. 3-22.
  50. Gabbani C. (2016). Scienza e senso comune: un primo sguardo d’insieme. In: Lavazza A. & Marraffa M., a cura di, La guerra dei mondi. Scienza e senso comune. Torino: Codice Edizioni, 2016, pp. 27-48.
  51. Gallagher S. (2001). The practice of mind. Theory, simulation or primary interaction? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 5/7: 83-108.
  52. Gallagher S. & Hutto D.D. (2008). Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In: Zlatev J., Racine T.P., Sinha C. & Itkonen E., editors, The Shared Mind: Perspectives on Intersubjectivity. Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2008, pp. 17-38.
  53. Gallese V. (2016). Finding the body in the brain. In: McLaughlin B. & Kornblith H.K., editors, Goldman and His Critics. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 2016, pp. 297-317.
  54. Gallese V., Keysers C. & Rizzolatti G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 9: 396-403.
  55. Heider F. (1958). The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York: Wiley (trad. it.: Psicologia delle relazioni interpersonali. Bologna: Il Mulino, 1972).
  56. Gallese V., Migone P. & Eagle M.N. (2006). La simulazione incarnata: i neuroni specchio, le basi neurofisiologiche dell’intersoggettività e alcune implicazioni per la psicoanalisi. Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, 40, 3: 543-580.
  57. Gallese V., Eagle M.N. & Migone P. (2007). Intentional attunement: Mirror neurons and the neural underpinnings of interpersonal relations. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 55, 1: 131-176. DOI: 10.1177/00030651070550010601
  58. Gammack G. (1982). Social work as uncommon sense. British Journal of Social Work, 12, 1: 3-22.
  59. Garb H.N. & Boyle P.A. (2014). Understanding why some clinicians use pseudoscientific methods. Findings from research on clinical judgment. In: Lilienfeld S.O., Lynn S.J. & Lohr J.M., editors, Science and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology. New York: Guilford, 2014, pp. 17-38.
  60. Gauker C. (2003). Words without Meaning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  61. Gendreau P., Irvine M. & Knight S. (1973). Evaluating response set styles on the MMPI with prisoners: Faking good adjustment and maladjustment. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 5, 2: 183-194.
  62. Gigerenzer G., Todd P.M. & the ABC Research Group (1999). Simple Heuristics that Make Us Smart. New York: Oxford University Press.
  63. Giosan C., Glovsky V. & Haslam N. (2001). The lay concept of “mental disorder”: A cross-cultural study. Transcultural Psychiatry, 38, 3: 317-332. DOI: 10.1177/136346150103800303
  64. Goldman A.I. (2006). Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  65. Gopnik A. (1996). Theories and modules: Creation myths, developmental realities, and Neurath’s boat. In: Carruthers & Smith, 1996, pp. pp. 169-83.
  66. Gordon R. (1995). Folk psychology as simulation. In: Davies M. & Stone T., editors, Folk psychology, Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 1995, pp. 60-73.
  67. Grice P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In: Cole P. & Morgan J.L., editors, Syntax and Semantics. Vol. 3: Speech Acts. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, 1975, pp. 41-58. (trad. it. Logica e conversazione. In: Sbisà M., a cura di, Gli atti linguistici. Milano: Feltrinelli, 1978, pp. 199-219).
  68. Hacking I. (1995). The looping effects of human kinds. In: Sperber D., Premack D. & Premack J.A., editors, Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary Debate New York: Oxford University Press, 1995, pp. 351-394.
  69. Haslam N. (2000). Psychiatric categories as natural kinds: Essentialist thinking about mental disorder. Social Research: An International Quarterly, 67: 1031-1058.
  70. Haslam N. (2002). Kinds of kinds: A conceptual taxonomy of psychiatric categories. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 9, 3: 203-217.
  71. Haslam N. (2005). Dimensions of folk psychiatry. Review of General Psychology, 9, 1: 35-47. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.9.1.35
  72. Haslam N. & Giosan C. (2002). The lay concept of “mental disorder” among American undergraduates. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 4: 479-485.
  73. Haslam N. & Ernst D. (2002). Essentialist beliefs about mental disorders. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21, 6: 628-644.
  74. Haslam N., Rothschild L. & Ernst D. (2004). Essentialism and entitativity: Structures of beliefs about the ontology of social categories. In: Yzerbyt V., Judd C.M. & Corneille O., editors, The Psychology of Group Perception: Perceived Variability, Entitativity, and Essentialism. London: Routledge, 2004, pp. 61-78.
  75. Haslam N., Ban L. & Kaufmann, L. (2007). Lay conceptions of mental disorder: The folk psychiatry model. Australian Psychologist, 42, 2: 129-137. DOI: 10.1080/00050060701280615
  76. Henley S. & Furnham A. (1988). The attribution of cure. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 4: 384-386.
  77. Hollingshead A.B. & Redlich F.C. (1958). Social Class and Mental Illness: Community Study. Wiley (trad. it.: Classi sociali e malattie mentali. Torino: Einaudi, 1958).
  78. Horwitz A.V. (2002). Creating Mental Illness. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  79. Horwitz A.V. & Wakefield J.C. (2007). The Loss of Sadness. How Psychiatry Transformed Normal Sorrow into Depressive Disorders. Foreword by Robert L. Spitzer. New York: Oxford University Press (trad. it.: La perdita della tristezza. Come la psichiatria ha trasformato la tristezza in depressione. Premessa di Mario Maj. Roma: L’Asino d’Oro, 2015).
  80. Hutto D. (2004). The limits of spectatorial folk psychology. Mind and Language, 19, 5: 548-573.
  81. Hutto D. (2008). Folk Psychological Narratives: The Sociocultural Basis of Understanding Reasons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  82. Hutto D.D. (2016). A reconciliation for the future of psychiatry: Both folk psychology and cognitive science. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 7, 12.
  83. Hutto D. & Ratcliffe M., editors (2007), Folk Psychology Re-assessed. Dordrecht: Springer.
  84. Ickes W.J. (2003). Everyday Mind Reading: Understanding What Other People Think and Feel. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.
  85. James W. (1907). Lecture V: Pragmatism and common sense. In: James W., Pragmatism. A New Name for Some Old Ways of Thinking. London: Longmans, Green and Co., 1907. Internet: https://en.wikisource.org/wiki/Pragmatism:_A_New_Name_for_Some_Old_Ways_of_Thinking#Lecture_V:_Pragmatism_and_Common_Sense.
  86. Jedlowski P. (1998). Il mondo in questione: Introduzione al pensiero sociologico. Roma: Carocci
  87. Jedlowski P. (2005). Un giorno dopo l’altro. La vita quotidiana fra esperienza e routine. Bologna: Il Mulino.
  88. Jorm A.F. (2000). Mental health literacy. Public knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 5: 396-401.
  89. Jorm A.F., Korten A.E., Jacomb P.A., Christensen H., Rodgers B. & Pollitt P. (1997a). “Mental health literacy”: A survey of the public’s ability to recognise mental disorders and their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. Medical Journal of Australia, 166, 4: 182-186.
  90. Jorm A.F., Korten A.E., Jacomb P.A., Christensen H., Rodgers B. & Pollitt P. (1997b). Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for depression and schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32, 3: 143-148. DOI: 10.1007/BF00794613
  91. Jorm A.F., Korten A.E., Jacomb P.A., Rodgers B., Pollitt P., Christensen H. & Henderson S. (1997c). Helpfulness of interventions for mental disorders: beliefs of health professionals compared with the general public. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 3: 233-237.
  92. Jorm A.F., Korten A.E., Rodgers B., Pollitt P., Jacomb P.A., Christensen H. & Jiao Z. (1997d). Belief systems of the general public concerning the appropriate treatments for mental disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32, 8: 468-473. DOI: 10.1007/BF00789141
  93. Joule R.V. & Beauvois J.L. (2015). La soumission librement consentie. Comment amener les gens à faire librement ce qu’ils doivent faire? Paris: PUF.
  94. Kahneman D. & Tversky A. (1982). The psychology of preferences. Scientific American, 246, 1: 160-173.
  95. Kahneman D. & Klein G. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. American Psychologist, 64, 6: 515-526.
  96. Kelley H.H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 2: 107-128.
  97. Kirk S.A., Wakefield J.C., Hsieh D., & Pottick K. (1999). Social context and social workers’ judgment of mental disorder. Social Service Review, 73, 1: 82-104. DOI: 10.1086/515798
  98. Lavazza A. & Marraffa M., a cura di, (2016). La guerra dei mondi. Scienza e senso comune. Torino: Codice Edizioni.
  99. Lavazza A. & Marraffa M. (2016a). Scienza e senso comune. Una guida per il lettore. In: Lavazza & Marraffa, 2016, pp. 4-24.
  100. Leslie A.M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 4: 412-426. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.412
  101. Lilienfeld S.O., Lynn J.L., Ruscio J. & Beyerstein B.L. (2010). 50 Great Myths of Popular Psychology: Shattering Widespread Misconceptions about Human Behavior. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell (trad it.: I grandi miti della psicologia popolare. Milano: Raffaello Cortina, 2011).
  102. Link B.G., Phelan J.C., Bresnahan M., Stueve A. & Pescosolido B.A. (1999). Public conceptions of mental illness: Labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 9: 1328-1333.
  103. MacCarthy B. & Furnham A. (1986). Patients’ conceptions of psychological adjustment in the normal population. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 25, 1: 43-50.
  104. Malle B.F. (1999). How people explain behavior: A new theoretical framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 1: 23-48.
  105. Mameli M. (2001). Mindreading, mindshaping, and evolution. Biology and Philosophy, 16: 597-628. DOI: 10.1023/A:1012203830990
  106. Matschinger H., & Angermeyer M.C. (1996). Lay beliefs about the causes of mental disorders: a new methodological approach. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31, 6: 309-315. DOI: 10.1007/BF00783418
  107. McGeer V. (2007). The regulative dimension of folk psychology. In: Hutto & Ratcliffe, 2007, pp. 138-156.
  108. McKeon P. & Carrick S. (1991). Public attitudes to depression: A national survey. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 8, 2: 116-121. DOI: 10.1017/S0790966700015020
  109. Morton A. (1996). Folk psychology is not a predictive device. Mind, 105, 417:119-37.
  110. Morton A. (2007). Folk psychology does not exist. In: Hutto & Ratcliffe, 2007, pp. 211-221.
  111. Morton A. (2009). Folk psychology. In: McLaughlin B., Beckermann A. & Walter S., editors, The Oxford handbook of philosophy of mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2009. pp. 809-824.
  112. Murphy D. (2006). Psychiatry in the Scientific Image. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  113. Nichols S. & Stich S.P. (2003). Mindreading: An Integrated Account of Pretence, Self-Awareness, and Understanding Other Minds. Oxford: Clarendon.
  114. Nisbett R. & Wilson T. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychology Review, 84, 3: 231-259.
  115. Nunnally J.C.Jr. (1961). Popular Conceptions of Mental Health: Their Development and Change. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  116. Perconti P. (2015). La prova del budino. Il senso comune e la nuova scienza cognitiva. Milano: Mondadori.
  117. Polanyi M. (2009). The Tacit Dimension. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press (trad. it.: La conoscenza inespressa. Roma: Armando, 2018).
  118. Ramsey G.V. & Seipp M. (1948). Attitudes and opinions concerning mental illness. Psychiatric Quarterly, 22, 3: 428-444. DOI: 10.1007/BF01572419
  119. Ratcliffe M. (2006). “Folk psychology” is not folk psychology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 5, 1: 31-52.
  120. Reid T. (1915) Selections from the Scottish Philosophy of Common Sense. Chicago, IL: Open Court.
  121. Ribeiro J., Franklin J., Fox K., Bentley K., Kleiman E., Chang B. & Nock M. (2016). Self-injurious thoughts and behaviors as risk factors for future suicide ideation, attempts, and death: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Medicine, 46, 2: 225-236. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291715001804
  122. Rippere V. (1980). Some historical dimensions of commonsense knowledge about depression and antidepressive behaviour. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18, 5: 373-385. DOI: 10.1016/0005-7967(80)90002-9
  123. Rizzolatti G. & Sinigaglia C. (2006). So quel che fai. Il cervello che agisce e i neuroni specchio Milano: Raffaello Cortina.
  124. Rothbart M. & Taylor M. (1992). Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds? In: Semin G.R. & Fiedler K., editors, Language, Interaction and Social Cognition. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1992, pp. 11-36.
  125. Ross L. & Nisbett R.E. (1991). The Person and the Situation. Perspectives of Social Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill (trad. it.: La persona e la situazione. Bologna: Il Mulino, 1998).
  126. Ryle G. (1946). Knowing how and knowing that: The presidential address. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 46, 1: 1-16.
  127. Schutz A. (1962). Common-sense and scientific interpretation of human action. In: Collected Papers. Vol. 1: The Problem of Social Reality. La Hague. NL: Martinus Nijhoff.
  128. Sellars W. (1956). Empiricism and the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1997 (trad. it.: Empirismo e filosofia della mente. Torino: Einaudi, 2004).
  129. Sellars W. (1962). Philosophy and the scientific image of man. In: Colodny R.G., Frontiers of Science and Philosophy. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press (trad. it.: La filosofia e l’immagine scientifica dell’uomo. Roma: Armando, 2007).
  130. Skinner B.F. (1974). About Behaviorism. New York: Knopf.
  131. Skinner B.F. (1972). Beyond Freedom and Dignity. London: Jonathan Cape.
  132. Smedslund J. (1989). What is psychologic. In: Inizio modulo
  133. Baker W.J., Hyland M.E., van Hezewijk R. & Terwee S., editors, Recent Trends in Theoretical Psychology. Vol. II: Proceedings of the Third Biennial Conference of the International Society for Theoretical Psychology, April 17-21, 1989. New York: Springer, 1990, pp 453-458. Fine modulo
  134. Spaulding S. (2018). How We Understand Others: Philosophy and Social Cognition. New York: Routledge.
  135. Stanghellini G. (2001). Psychopathology of common sense. Philosophy Psychiatry & Psychology, 8, 8, 2/3: 201-218.
  136. Star S. (1955). “The public’s ideas about mental illness”. Paper presented at: Annual Meeting of the National Association for Mental Health, Indianapolis, November 5, 1955. Internet: www.norc.org/PDFs/publications/StarS_Publics_Ideas_1955.pdf.
  137. Stich S.P. (1983). From Folk Psychology to Cognitive Science: The Case Against Belief. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (trad. it.: Dalla psicologia del senso comune alla scienza cognitiva. Bologna: Il Mulino, 1994).
  138. Stich S.P. & Nichols S. (1992). Folk psychology: Simulation or tacit theory? Mind & Language, 7, 1/2: 35-71.
  139. Tversky A. & Kahneman D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 4157: 1124-1131.
  140. Wakefield J.C. (1992a). Disorder as harmful dysfunction: A conceptual critique of DSM-III-R’s definition of mental disorder. Psychological Review, 99, 2: 232-47. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.232
  141. Wakefield J.C. (1992b). The concept of mental disorder: On the boundary between biological facts and social values. American Psychologist, 47, 3: 373-88.
  142. Wakefield J.C. (2000). Spandrels, vestigial organs, and such: Reply to Murphy and Woolfolk’s “The harmful dysfunction analysis of mental disorder”. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 7, 4: 253-269.
  143. Wakefield J.C. (2004). Realtà e valori nel concetto di disturbo mentale: il disturbo come disfunzione dannosa. Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, 38, 4: 439-464.
  144. Wampold B.E. & Imel Z.E. (2015). The Great Psychotherapy Debate: The Evidence for What Makes Psychotherapy Work. New York: Routledge (trad. it.: Il grande dibattito in psicoterapia. L’evidenza della ricerca scientifica avanzata applicata alla clinica. Roma: Sovera, 2017).
  145. Wellman H. (1990). The Child’s Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  146. Wilson T.D. (2003). Knowing when to ask: Introspection and the adaptive unconscious. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 9/10: 131-140.
  147. Wilson T.D., Hull J.G. & Johnson J. (1981). Awareness and self-perception: Verbal reports on internal states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1: 53-71. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.40.1.5
  148. Wilkes K.V. (1981). Functionalism, psychology and the philosophy of mind. Philosophical Topics, 12, 1: 147-67.
  149. Wilkes K.V. (1984). Pragmatics in science and theory in common sense. Inquiry, 27, 1/4: 339-361. DOI: 10.1080/00201748408602024
  150. Wilkes K.V. (1991). The relationship between scientific psychology and common-sense psychology. Synthese, 89: 15-39. DOI: 10.1007/BF0041397.
  151. Wimmer H. & Perner J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 1: 103-128. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
  152. Wittgenstein L. (1969). On Certainty. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell (trad. it.: Della certezza: L’analisi filosofica del senso comune. Torino: Einaudi, 1978).
  153. Zangwill N. (2005). The normativity of the mental. Philosophical Explorations, 8, 1: 1-19. DOI: 10.1080/1386979042000336126
  154. Zawidzki T. W. (2008). The function of folk psychology: Mind reading or mind shaping? Philosophical Explorations, 11, 3: 193-210. DOI: 10.1080/13869790802239235.

Andrea Angelozzi, Folk psychiatry. La psichiatria fra immagine scientifica e psichiatria popolare in "PSICOTERAPIA E SCIENZE UMANE" 3/2022, pp 431-546, DOI: 10.3280/PU2022-003004